adjective

a constituent with the feature composition: [+N, +V, –F] modifying nouns, e.g. mad in mad cow. These constituents cannot have nominal complements, their semantically nominal complement must appear as a Prepositional Phrase with the rescue strategy of of-insertion.

adverb

a constituent with the feature composition [+N, +V, –F] used to modify a verb (as in everything went smoothly) or a sentence (as in Unfortunately, I did not pass the first exam). In this approach adverbs and adjectives belong to the same category, the difference between them being what they modify.

clause

a structure containing a (visible or invisible) subject and a predicate.

comparative form of adjectives

this form is used for comparison to a higher (or in the case of less lower) degree when two constituents are compared: He is taller than I am. This sentence contains inflectional comparative, but there is another, periphrastic way of comparison: This car is more expensive than that one.

complementary distribution

two constituents are in complementary distribution if one of them never appears in any of the environments where the other appears. If two constituents are in complementary distribution it indicates that they compete for the same structural position. E.g. we cannot have both an inflectional ending and a modal auxiliary in the same clause as these two occupy the head position within an IP, thus the ungrammaticality of *She can dances.

degree adverb

a subclass of adverbs which specifies the degree to which some property applies, e.g. very and extremely. Feature composition: [+F, +N, +V]

determiner

the head of a Determiner Phrase, a closed class item taking an NP complement defining its definiteness. Feature composition: [+F, –N, +V]

distribution

the set of positions that the grammar determines to be possible for a given category. Words that distribute in the same way will belong to the same categories, words that distribute differently will belong to different categories.

[±F]

one of the three basic binary features on which all categories can be defined. With the help of these features we can explain why we have the categories that we do and also describe how these categories are related. With the help of the three binary features we can predict what kinds of categories are possible in human language, we can give an exclusive list of them. [±F] is a feature used to distinguish between functional and thematic categories. [–F] categories have thematic content and [+F] categories do not. The categories with [+F] feature are the following: inflections, complementisers, determiners and degree adverbs. Certain categories are unspecified for the [±F] feature, see underspecification.

inflection

(a) a morpheme added to the end of words of a given category in sentence structure as required by the given structure, e.g. s in Peter like s his dog or er in Peter is clever er than Tony.

(b) the head of an Inflectional Phrase. It can be realised as a modal auxiliary or a zero agreement morpheme. Information about tense can be found in a separate vP directly under IP.

inflectional comparison

the comparative and superlative forms of the adjective are expressed with the help of the inflectional endings er and est. E.g. hungrier/hungriest. See also periphrastic comparison.

lexical entry

a collection of the idiosyncratic properties of lexical items.

morpheme

the smallest meaningful unit. Words can be made up of one or more morphemes. See also bound morpheme, free morpheme.

[±N]

one of the three basic binary features on which all categories can be defined. With the help of these features we can explain why we have the categories that we do and also describe how these categories are related. With the help of the three binary features we can predict what kinds of categories are possible in human language, we can give an exclusive list of them. Since we want to define verbs and nouns as polar opposites the abstract binary features [±N] and [±V] were introduced, though obviously they do not mean noun and verb and are used to define other categories besides nouns and verbs. A property linked to the [–N] feature is the ability to have a nominal complement. The categories with [+N] feature are the following: a. thematic: nouns, adjectives; b. functional: determiners, degree adverbs; unspecified for the [F] value: post-determiners, measure nouns.

superlative form of adjectives

comparison to a higher (or in the case of least lower) degree when there are more than two agents involved: He is the tallest of us. The periphrastic way of forming the superlative is with the help of most: He is the most sophisticated man I have ever met.

[±V]

one of the three basic binary features on which all categories can be defined. With the help of these features we can explain why we have the categories that we do and also describe how these categories are related. With the help of the three binary features we can predict what kinds of categories are possible in human language, we can give an exclusive list of them. Since we want to define verbs and nouns as polar opposites the abstract binary features [±N] and [±V] were introduced, though obviously they do not mean noun and verb and are used to define other categories besides nouns and verbs. The categories with [±V] feature are the following: a. thematic: verbs, prepositions; b. functional: inflections, degree adverbs, aspectual auxiliaries; unspecified for the [F] value: aspectual auxiliaries, post-determiners.

Basic English Syntax with Exercises

1.3.5.3 Degree Adverbs

So far we have looked at auxiliary verbs, which accompany verbs, and determiners, which accompany nouns, classifying these as functional equivalents of the categories they accompany. The obvious choice for functional adjectives, therefore, are the degree adverbs that accompany them:

(166)aso light
btoo heavy
cas thick (as a brick)

Thus we might categorise these elements as [+F, +N, +V].

It is a complex, but interesting question as to what counts as a degree adverb. Firstly, these elements are used primarily to indicate the degree to which the state or property expressed by an adjective holds of something. But there are a number of elements that do this, not all of which seem to behave the same:

(167)atoo strong
bvery fast
cquite real
dextremely tiring

Some of these degree modifiers are in complementary distribution with each other, indicating that they belong to the same category:

(168)a*too so tall
b*so as wide
c*as too long

However, others are not in complementary distribution:

(169)aso very boring
bquite as fragile
cas extremely frustrating

This would suggest that not all of these words should be categorised as degree adverbs, that is, as words with [+F, +N, +V] categorial features. Given that normal adverbs can be used to modify adjectives, some of the cases in (167) can simply be taken as adverbs, especially those that are formed from adjectives by the ly morpheme:

(170)ahe obviously leftobviously tired
bthey certainly metcertainly irregular
cwe wanted it extremelyextremely tough

Others however are more difficult to categorise. Words like very do not appear to be able to be used as typical adverbs, modifying verbs or sentences, but are restricted to modifying adjectives as are the degree adverbs:

(171)*he flexed his muscles very

Besides distributional properties, degree adverbs also have other properties that unify them. For example, it is typical for a degree adverb to appear alongside a clausal element which follows the adjective being modified:

(172)aso fat [that he couldn’t do up the buttons]
btoo far [to walk]
cas stupid [as they come]

This clause specifies the bounds to which the degree of the property expressed by the modified adjective is given. Note that plain adverb modifiers of adjectives do not appear with such limiting clauses:

(173)a*very tired [that he had to rest]
b*extremely big [to get through the door]
c*quite famous [as I am]

There is an interpretation in which these kind of constructions are not ungrammatical. However, this is where the following clauses are associated with the adjectives or even the whole clause rather than the degree modifiers:

(174)ahe was tired, [(so) that he had to rest]
bthe sofa was big [to get through the door]
che is famous, [as I am] (in formal English: as am I)

This is another reason to consider these words to belong to different categories.

Other words which behave as degree adverbs both distributionally and in that they can be accompanied by a limiting clause are the comparative and superlative adverbs more and most:

(175)aso fanaticalmore fanatical*so more fanatical
bas wonderfulmost wonderful*as most wonderful
cmore predictable [than I am]
dmost regrettable [of all]

Although the accompanying element to most does not look much like a clause, its interpretation is of all the things that are regrettable, which is more clause like. These observations also lead us to consider the inflectional comparative and superlative:

(176)aso nicenicer*so nicer
bas talltallest*as tallest
ccuter [than I am]
dstrongest [of all]

Clearly, these behave exactly like the periphrastic constructions, and hence would seem to involve a degree adverb. The obvious choice would be the comparative and superlative morphemes themselves, which would suggest an analysis similar to what was proposed for verbal inflections: the comparative and superlative are independent lexical elements which are inserted into an expression separately into the degree adverb position and then by a syntactic process become attached to the adjective:

(177)a
b

Below, we can see a selection of lexical entries for degree adverbs:

(178)socategory:[+F, +N, +V]
subcat:[adjectival]
morecategory:[+F, +N, +V]
subcat:[adjectival]
-ercategory:[+F, +N, +V]
subcat:[adjectival]