arguments

the participants minimally involved in an action defined by the predicate. The complements and the subject, the latter also called an external argument.

constituent

a linguistic expression that functions as a unit in grammatical structure. A group of words that undergo syntactic processes together.

determiner phrase (DP)

a phrase headed by a central determiner or the possessive ’s morpheme. The complement of a DP is an NP, the specifier the DP the possessive ending attaches to.

extraposition

a constituent (PP, CP) moved from the phrase where it belongs to a sentence final position: The rumour t has been circulating [that we will have an oral exam this semester].

grammar

(a) a (finite) set of rules which tell us how to recognise the infinite number of expressions that constitute the language that we speak. (b) a linguistic hypothesis about these rules.

language

a system that enables people who speak it to produce and understand linguistic expressions.

Move α

move anything anywhere. Further restrictions on movement come from factors independent from the formulation of the movement rule.

object

a DP complement immediately following the verb. It can move to the subject position in passive sentences. See also direct object, indirect object.

object position

the specifier position of VP.

passive voice

the subject of the passive sentence is interpreted as the object of the verb.

phrase

a group of words that can undergo syntactic operations (e.g. movement) as a unit.

preposition phrase (PP)

a phrase headed by a preposition. It usually takes a DP complement but certain types of CPs can also appear in the complement position of PPs. PPs themselves can be complements of different constituents such as verbs, nouns and adjectives.

specifier position

a position defined by X-bar Theory. The specifier is sister to X', daughter of XP. It is a phrasal position, the nature of the phrase depends on what it is the specifier of. E.g. the specifier of IP is the subject, the specifier of DP is the possessor in possessive structures.

Structure Preservation Principle

no movement can alter the basic X-bar nature of structure, structures are projected from the lexicon at all levels.

subject position

the position where subjects appear in the tree. The base position of the subject depends on its theta role. Agents and experiencers are generated in Spec,vP. Theme subjects appear in Spec,VP. These positions are not Case positions, so the subjects move to the canonical subject position, Spec, IP.

subject–auxiliary inversion

a descriptive cover term for the reverse order of the subject and the auxiliary in questions like Can you dance?, see also I-to-C movement.

topic

an element appearing in front of the subject with a special interpretation (something like ‘as far as topic is concerned’). Topics have either already been mentioned before in a conversation or can be interpreted as easily accessible due to the context.

topicalisation

a process which moves an element interpreted as a topic to the front of the sentence.

yes–no question

a question that can be answered either with yes or no, formed either by inverting the auxiliary with the subject as in Would you like to go to the cinema? or the insertion of dummy do as in Did you enjoy the performance?.

Basic English Syntax with Exercises

3.2.1 Move α

Once the idea has been put forward that things can move about within a sentence, we can see that it can be applied to a lot of linguistic phenomena:

(50)athe water was wasted
bis this the end?
cthis conclusion, virtually no one has ever come to
dthe plans were released for the new car park

In the first case in (50) we have a passive sentence in which the subject is interpreted as the object: the water was what was wasted, not what did the wasting. We might claim that in this case the object moves from object position into subject position:

(51)

In (50b) we have what is termed a yes–no question, as it may be answered with a simple “yes” or “no”. These questions typically involve subject–auxiliary inversion, in which the auxiliary verb and the subject appear to switch places. A more current view of the inversion process is that the auxiliary moves to a position to the left of the subject:

(52)

(50c) involves topicalisation, a process which moves an element interpreted as a topic to the front of the sentence. A topic is typically something that has already been mentioned before in a conversation, or can be interpreted as easily accessible in a conversation due to the context. Consider the sentence in (50c), it is obvious that the conclusion mentioned must have been a part of the preceding discussion and that it has not just been newly introduced. We may analyse this sentence as:

(53)

Finally in (50d) we see another kind of movement which appears to split a constituent across the structure. The preposition phrase for the new car park, is clearly related to the noun plans. Indeed, this PP is the complement of the noun:

(54)the plans for the new car park

However, the PP appears to have been moved out of the subject DP into the sentence final position. This process is known as extraposition:

(55)

As we saw in the previous section grammatical processes should be stated as simply and generally as possible if we are to provide a theory that can cover the basic fact of language learnability. This would argue against an approach to movement in which we provide many movement rules designed to capture the specific facts about individual movements. Instead we should follow the example of the previous section and provide a small number of general rules which have a wide applicability.

In fact the general assumption is that there is just one movement rule, usually called Move α which can be stated as:

(56)Move α
Move anything anywhere.

This might not seem a very wise kind of rule to allow in a grammar as it would seem to sanction complete chaos and English does not appear to be anything near chaotic in its grammatical organisation.

The rule in (56) indeed would sanction chaos if this were all there was to say about movement. However there is a good deal more to be said. Let us take X-bar structures into consideration. When an object moves to subject position in a passive construction it is moving from one DP position to another: complement of the verb to specifier position. Simplifying somewhat, we might suppose the following analysis:

(57)

Here the main verb wasted takes its argument in the specifier position of its own phrase. This phrase is in turn the complement of the auxiliary verb was. The argument moves from the specifier of the lower verb to that of the higher one.

The structure we end up with is one that is perfectly compatible with X-bar principles. In other words, the movement seems not to have radically altered the structure. Suppose we assume a restriction on all movements, that they cannot alter structures in a way that would violate basic X-bar principles:

(58)Structure Preservation Principle
no movement can alter the basic X-bar nature of structure

This would rule out immediately a very large class of movements possible under the assumption of (56).

The important point to recognise is that the assumption of (56) and the imposition of a restriction such as (58) offer a far simpler and general theory of what can move where than would a theory that was made up of lots of specific rules telling us what can move where and under what conditions in particular cases. Of course, (56) and (58) together still do not constitute a particularly accurate theory of movement and there are still many movements allowed under these assumptions that do not actually occur. However, even if we add five or ten more restrictions of the kind in (58) we would still have a more general theory of movement than the literally hundreds of movement rules that would be required to describe specific cases of movements. We will see that the number of restrictions required to capture the majority of facts about movement is surprisingly small.